Here is the Psychology of Women and Gender in the 21st
Century
Psychology of women has come a long way from Helene
Deutsch's (1944) psychoanalytic writing and the century-long tradition of
gender differences research in psychology. Today it has a vibrant foundation of
three decades of contemporary research and a promising future built on this
foundation, yet with many questions unanswered. Psychology of women today
interweaves three strands: (a) psychological research focused specifically on
women, their psychological functioning, and women-associated issues such as
rape; (b) the psychology of gender, incorporating traditional research on
gender differences but expanding beyond that to examine gender as a stimulus
variable; and (c) feminist psychology, rooted in feminist theory and an
articulated set of values that emphasizes equality of opportunity for women. We
incorporate all three of these strands in our review.
Introduction

Here we will consider four major areas that represent the
current status of the psychology of women and, we believe, will chart the
future of psychology of women: theories in the psychology of women; the
intersection of gender with race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and disability;
research methods in the psychology of women; and applications of research on
the psychology of women in the areas of public policy, forensic practice, and
the practice of psychotherapy and counseling.
Theories
in the Psychology of Women
Few "grand theories" have been proposed in the
field of psychology of women; one of the few is gender schema theory (Bem,
1981), reviewed below. This is not to say that the field is atheoretical.
Rather than building grand theories, researchers have concentrated on rewriting
and redefining the androcentric theories upon which psychology was established
and building models to deal with more circumscribed problems. Feminist theory,
often borrowing from the humanities, has been a rich source as well. Here we
review each of these approaches, with examples.
Gender
Schema Theory
Bem's (1981) gender schema theory reflects the cognitive
revolution in psychology over the last several decades and applies this
approach to understanding the development of gender stereotyping. In cognitive
psychology, "schema" generally refers to a cognitive structure,
developed from prior learning, that is used when filtering and interpreting new
information. For Bem, a gender schema is a person's general knowledge framework
about gender, with which information is processed and organized based on
gender-linked associations. Children gradually form a gender schema as they
learn their culture's network of associations with gender. Moreover, the gender
schema becomes linked to self-concept so that children, as part of their
motivation to become "good" girls or boys, engage in the
gender-appropriate behavior specified by the gender schema.
Many studies support gender schema theory. In testing the
theory, Bem categorized participants into gender-schematic persons (masculine
males and feminine females, as determined by Bem's test of androgyny [1974])
and gender-aschematic people (androgynous males and androgynous females). She
argued that gender-schematic people are more likely to engage in
gender-schematic processing of information, whereas gender-aschematic people
engage in it less. In a free-recall test of a list of words, gender-schematic
people were more likely to cluster the words by gender (e.g., gorilla, bull,
trousers), than gender-aschematic people were. Reaction-time data indicated
that gender-schematic people, when responding "me" or "not
me," processed schema-consistent attributes faster than they processed
schema-inconsistent attributes. Martin and Halverson (1983) found that, if
five- and six-year-old children were shown pictures of children engaging in
stereotype-consistent or stereotype-inconsistent activities, a week later the
children made errors in recall of the stereotype-inconsistent pictures,
recalling them as stereotype-consistent. That is, if they had seen a picture of
two girls boxing, they remembered that they had seen two boys boxing.
Gender schema theory is compelling for a number of
reasons, one being that it explains why gender stereotypes are so resistant to
change -- our gender schema simply filters out stereotype-inconsistent
information. The theory has also been criticized but the reasons are too
technical to go into here (e.g., Spence & Helmreich, 1981). From our point
of view, the main limitation of the theory is that it is exclusively cognitive.
Emotion is reemerging as a prominent construct within
psychology in general, and in the 21st century we look toward theories of
gender that integrate cognition (thought) and affect (emotion). For example, the
expanding literature on self-concepts and self-strategies (see review by Wood,
1989) is just waiting to be incorporated into research on gender. As we noted,
gender schema theory provides a cognitive explanation for why gender
stereotypes are so difficult to change. However, another plausible, and very
exciting explanation might suggest that some people are motivated to maintain
their gender stereotypes. For instance, there may be times when people
accentuate or draw attention to their own gender-stereotyped traits, if doing
so makes them feel good about themselves. People may feel more positively about
themselves and their own gender when they degrade the other gender.
On the other hand, some people do indeed work on reducing
their stereotyped attitudes about gender, but what motivates them to work so
hard at this? Maybe there are situations in which gender stereotypes become so
personally limiting and/or defeating that people want to reject them? How do
they do this? Integrating self-concept research into research about gender
presents the opportunity to determine motivational factors underlying why
gender is such a salient interpersonal characteristic for both women and men,
and why its importance is so immutable.
Specific
Models
Here we will provide two examples of research building
specific theoretical models to deal with specific questions.
Stereotypes and power. Susan Fiske (1993) has proposed a
model of the ways in which power and stereotypes influence each other. Two
processes are involved: (a) Stereotyping exerts control or power over people,
pressuring them to conform; therefore, stereotyping maintains the status quo.
(b) Powerful people tend to stereotype less powerful people far more than the
reverse. Given that gender is an important status or power variable, you can
read "men" for "powerful people" and "women" for
"less powerful people." The theory is even broader than that, though,
and extends to other categories such as ethnic groups.
Let's consider the first process, in which gender stereotyping
exerts control of males and females. Stereotypes can be prescriptive -- that
is, they say how people of a certain group should behave. Adolescent boys
should excel at athletics. Girls should not be aggressive. If one fails to meet
the demands of such stereotypes, the penalties can be severe, such as social
rejection by the peer group. Stereotypes, therefore, exert control over people.
Turning to the second process, the powerful group (men)
tends to stereotype the less powerful group (women) more than the reverse. Less
powerful people generally are motivated to pay attention to the idiosyncrasies
of powerful people because those powerful people control outcomes for the less
powerful. Servants know a great many details about their employers and their preferences,
for example, but the reverse is unlikely. Powerful people pay less attention to
others and consequently rely on simple stereotypes. Powerful people pay less
attention to the less powerful in part because the less powerful have little
control over them.
Fiske (1993) has conducted many clever experiments to
test various aspects of her theory. In one study, undergraduates were given the
power to evaluate the summer job applications of high school students. Some
undergraduates were given more power in the final decision and others were
given less power. The students who were given more power actually paid less
attention to the applicants, consistent with Fiske's model.
Fiske's model and research were influential in an
important Supreme Court case, Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins. Ann Hopkins was
denied partnership in the prestigious accounting firm of Price Waterhouse.
Compared with her male colleagues who were also being considered for
partnership, she worked more billable hours, was well liked by clients, and brought
in millions of dollars in accounts. She was denied partnership not because her
performance was inadequate (it was in fact superb by objective standards), but
rather because she was not considered feminine. Stereotype violation, in short,
was used as grounds for the denial of promotion.
Based on Fiske's model, we can understand how stereotypes
operated at several levels in this case. Men were in power at Price Waterhouse
and women were outnumbered. Therefore, the powerful men were likely to hold
stereotyped expectations about the women and to pay less attention to their
individual details, such as their qualifications. Hopkins, by being a
successful woman in a male-dominated profession was a stereotype violator and
received punishment for it.
The Supreme Court ruled in favor of Hopkins, and the
brief filed by the American Psychological Association, reporting Susan Fiske's
research, was highly influential in the decision.
Men who sexually aggress against women. Some theorists
argue that too much rape research has focused on women who have been raped,
when the real emphasis should be on the aggressors. Only by gaining a
scientific understanding of them will we be able to reduce the occurrence of
rape. Neil Malamuth and colleagues (1991) developed a model of the factors that
predispose a man to engage in sexual coercion with women and then tested it
against data obtained from a large, national sample of male college students.
According to their model -- and the data supported it -- four factors predispose
a man to engage in sexual coercion: (a) Hostile home environment -- Violence
between parents or battering or sexually abusing the child increase the chances
that the boy will engage in sexual coercion. (b) Delinquency -- Being involved
in delinquency leads a boy to associate with delinquent peers who, for example,
encourage hostile attitudes and rationalizations for committing crimes and
reward a tough, aggressive image. (c) Sexual promiscuity -- Often in the
context of the delinquent group, the young man comes to believe that sexual
conquests bring him status within the peer group, and coercion may seem to be a
reasonable way to achieve his goals. (d) A hostile masculine personality --
This personality constellation involves deep hostility toward women together
with negatively defined, exaggerated masculinity--masculinity characterized as
rejecting anything feminine, such as nurturance, and emphasizing power,
control, and macho characteristics.
Understanding the factors that make some men rape-prone
will be crucial for intervention programs. What other factors might be related
to men sexually aggressing against women, and might these and other factors
interact or lead to one another in a causal sequence? How could such models be
tested?
Feminist
Theory
Research and theory about the psychology of women has
been heavily influenced by feminist theory (e.g., Jaggar & Rothenberg,
1993; Tong, 1989), much of which has originated in the humanities. An early
example was Bem's (1974) theorizing and research on androgyny, which was
preceded by earlier writings in the humanities (Heilbrun, 1973). We have no
doubt that this pattern will continue into the 21st century, as developments in
feminist theory are made in the humanities, and then translated by
psychologists into theory that is testable with empirical data. Here we offer
one recent example.
Nita McKinley and Janet Hyde (1996) developed the
Objectified Body Conscious (OBC) Scale to measure women's experience of their
own bodies. The research began with the writings of feminist theorists, who
argue that the female body is constructed as an object of male desire and
exists to receive the gaze of the male (Spitzack, 1990). The result is that
women experience objectified body consciousness (McKinley, 1995). OBC consists
of three components: (a) body surveillance, in which the woman or girl views
her own body as if an external onlooker and constantly evaluates it; (b)
internalization of cultural standards that specify the ideal female body and
the ideal for beauty; because these standards are internalized, conformity
seems to be a matter of personal choice rather than external pressure, and
shame results when one fails to control one's weight or appearance; and (c)
beliefs that one can control one's appearance and weight, which can lead to
unhealthy behaviors and eating disorders. Based on this feminist theorizing,
McKinley and Hyde (1996) developed the OBC scale to measure these three
components. Their data showed that high scores on these scales were associated
with disordered eating. Now the scales are available to those doing further
research on this important topic.
One of the most exciting aspects of the psychology of
women is that it is part of the interdisciplinary field of women's studies. As
such, there are rich opportunities to learn from developments in other
disciplines and in turn to use psychology to build on those developments.
Gender and Its Intersection with Race/Ethnicity,
Disability, and Sexual Orientation
One of the key tenets of feminist theory is that gender
does not act alone, but rather acts jointly with race/ethnicity, disability,
and sexual orientation, in influencing an individual's life experience. Here we
provide examples of some of this work that looks at the conjoint influence of
gender and these other status characteristics.
Gender
and Race/Ethnicity
We can see the intersection of gender with race/ethnicity
most directly if we examine variations in gender roles across ethnic groups in
the United States. For example, the evidence shows that some North American
Indian tribes had a system of egalitarian gender roles, in which separate but
equally valued tasks were assigned to women and men and women had a strong role
in government (Blackwood, 1984). The very work that is considered acceptable
for women or men can be determined by one's ethnic group.
Among Asian Americans, educational attainment is highly
valued and both males and females are expected to meet high standards. Asian
American women graduate from college at a higher rate than white men do (Hyde,
1996). There is no view that women are less worthy of education. In this case,
one's gendered aspirations are defined by one's ethnic group.
As these two examples indicate, we cannot begin to
understand the impact of gender roles on girls and women unless we consider
ethnicity simultaneously.
Gender
and Disability
Few individual characteristics rival gender in their
salience or impact in social situations, but disability is one of them. It is
surprising that only a small body of research exists on the interaction of
gender and disability; we believe that this literature will continue to expand
in the next decade. Michelle Fine and Adrienne Asch (1988) have edited an
interdisciplinary volume on this topic. They commented on the discrepancy
between feminists strivings for independence and disabled womens assumed
dependence. This misconception may be one reason why disability and gender have
not been extensively researched within the psychology of women.
Despite the scarcity of research, some areas have been
explored. For example, an analysis of autobiographies written by blind people
revealed that blind women were more likely than blind men to seek jobs within
the blind community, teaching and counseling other blind people (Asch &
Sacks, 1983). Furthermore, blind women were less likely than blind men to refer
to intimate relationships, but more likely to show concern about the visible
features of their disabilities.
Another line of research on women and disability has
focussed attention on womens experiences of sexuality and mothering. Asrael
(1982) articulated the discouragement and difficulty that disabled women face
when deciding to have children, and then offered a useful model within which
disabled women are described as working with a team of professionals to plan
their pregnancies and deliveries. This compelling idea highlights one of this
areas complexities. For example, would this approach be appropriate for all
women with disabilities? Embedded within this research topic is a challenge to
recognize differences and commonalities among peoples experiences who have
different disabilities.
Most of what we know in this area is descriptive in nature
and is based on interviews with disabled women. Although these studies are rich
with detail and are an important place to start, the underlying social and
psychological processes related to womens experiences will be identified only
when researchers apply psychological theory to this domain.
Disabled women (and men) have been described as members
of an }invisible” population. Although this description is true in general,
some disabilities are more invisible than others and have escaped the attention
of researchers to an even greater extent. Examples of these invisible
disabilities are learning disabilities, mental illness, and some physical or
sensory disabilities. These individuals who can }pass” as not disabled at first
glance may have unique experiences and frustrations that are not socially
recognized or validated. Researchers may gain insight into the nature of gender
and stigma associated with disability by focussing attention on how these women
choose to navigate through life labelled as disabled or not.
Another area that is rich with research possibility
concerns public attitudes about women with disabilities. Are women with
disabilities evaluated more negatively than disabled men when they need to
advocate for themselves? What attributions are made of their emotional
behavior? In what ways do physical atypicalities influence interpersonal
behaviors among women with disabilities, and how do these women deal with
extreme standards of beauty and femininity that may seem contradictory to their
disabilities? Approaching the topic from a more positive stance, are women with
disabilities more capable of challenging gender stereotypes or expectations
because of their often marginalized position? The answers to these questions
are unknown, and the questions continue to emerge. Psychology of women has its
work cut out for itself in the arena of understanding the complex relationships
between gender and disability.
Sexual
Orientation
Issues of sexual orientation have personally touched the
lives of many within the field of psychology of women and part of the research
attention afforded this topic can be traced to this interest. However, perhaps
more powerfully, psychology of women has a strong tradition of documenting and
validating the diversity in human experience. It is from this stance that
psychology of women has tackled the very complex issues related to sexual
orientation.
Like no other area of academe, psychology of women has
sought to describe and validate the experiences of lesbians (Boston Lesbian
Psychologies Collective, 1987; Greene & Herek, 1994; Davis, Cole, &
Rothblum, 1996; Weinstock & Rothblum, 1996). Some stereotypes have been
shattered, but perhaps more importantly, it has become clear that there is no
single lesbian experience. One important part of past research is that
psychologists have recognized and documented the challenges facing lesbians who
are building and maintaining relationships without the support of family,
friends, and/or social institutions.
Research has also focussed on the qualities of lesbian
relationships themselves. More specifically, these relationships offer one of
the only opportunities to witness the dynamics of intimate relationships
without the inherent inequalities introduced by pairing women and men. Although
most lesbian relationships are more egalitarian than heterosexual relationships
(Peplau, Cochran, Rock, & Padesky, 1978), one finding that was surprising
to some was the existence of violence within some lesbian relationships (Brand
& Kidd, 1986). This finding is particularly relevant to future research in
this area, in that investigators will continue to disentangle the network of
status and power that exists between people even in relationships where gender
is equalized.
Issues of sexual orientation are not limited to
lesbianism. Future research will focus on bisexual women. Bisexuality has often
been brushed aside, without having an identity of its own, but gradually it is
becoming less marginalized. Bisexuality offers a unique situation in which women
have the flexibility to alternate between }heterosexual” and }homosexual”
experiences depending on whom theyre currently dating. The psychological and
social complexities that accompany this situation, and the nature of a bisexual
identity or identities, will be illuminated in future research (Ault, 1996;
Weinberg, Williams, & Pryor, 1994).
Finally, research on sexual orientation will continue to
address issues related to social policy. For example, one line of research
shows that children are not disadvantaged as a result of being raised by
lesbian rather than heterosexual couples (Flaks, Ficher, Masterpasqua, &
Joseph, 1995; Patterson, 1992). This type of research may be influential in
adoption cases where concern is raised regarding whether lesbian couples are
fit to be parents. Furthermore, through research, lifestyles that may seem
marginal to some people, become more normalized and acceptable. There is great
power in showing that women who are lesbian or bisexual are not necessarily
different from heterosexual women on variables other than sexual orientation.
Research
Methods in the Psychology of Women
Researchers within psychology of women have adopted,
adapted, and cultivated a wide variety of research methods. Part of this
diversity has grown out of the challenges feminist researchers encounter as
they search for methods that will effectively answer their research questions
without contradicting their feminist ideals. The selection and application of
research methods is a series of tradeoffs because each has its strengths and
weaknesses, and none is a methodological panacea. The tradeoffs typically center
around two fundamental characteristics of any research endeavor: (a) whether
qualitative or quantitative data are collected, and (b) the context in which
the data were collected. For clarity, we will discuss these issues separately
although they are intertwined.
Quantitative
and Qualitative Research
Regarding the first issue, data can be either qualitative
or quantitative. Qualitative data come in the form of verbal description and
are usually summarized in text, whereas quantitative data are in the form of
numbers and are analyzed via statistical procedures. One of the most well-known
studies of the qualitative type was conducted on womens moral reasoning by
Carol Gilligan (1982). Gilligan conducted a series of elaborate interviews with
women who were making the decision of whether or not to have an abortion. She
designed her research in response to the androcentric theory of moral reasoning
advanced by Kohlberg (1969) in which he pronounced women to be less mature in
their moral reasoning than men. In contrast, Gilligan concluded that women were
not deficient in their abilities to reason morally, but rather, reasoned from
different premises than did men. According to Gilligan, men mainly use
considerations of justice in their moral reasoning, whereas women attend more
to relationships and the imperative to care for others. It is easy to
understand how Gilligan's interview-based methodology is congruent with the
feminist goal of documenting and validating the lived experiences of women.
This approach can uncover detailed information about individuals' experiences
by taking into account the contexts in which they live and the intricacies of
their lives.
Qualitative methods such as these can definitely
contribute a great deal to the field. However, due to the elaborate processes
by which qualitative data are collected and synthesized, these accounts
typically represent the experiences of only a small number of people; thus, the
virtue of this method is also its vice. Because of the rich detail and concern
for individuals unique experiences, it is often difficult to draw general
conclusions.
Investigators using quantitative methods, in contrast,
frame and seek to answer research questions using statistical techniques. For
example, participants may be asked to report their attitudes on a scale from
say, 1 to 7, and by doing this they are quantifying their attitudes rather than
describing them in their own words. Although there is considerable utility (for
example, one can collect data on a large, random sample) in being able to
summarize people's attitudes in this manner, this benefit comes at a cost. It
is likely, if not inevitable, that part of individuals richness and depth are
lost when they are asked to report information in this way. In summarizing the distinction
between qualitative and quantitative methods then, we might say that whereas
the benefit of qualitative data is depth, the benefit of quantitative data is
breadth.
Contextual
and Design Factors in Research
Another important methodological distinction is between
naturalistic and laboratory studies. Naturalistic investigations are conducted
in the environments where people live, work, and play, rather than in
laboratories. Because feminists stress the importance of context, many would
suggest that it is not optimal to strip these important factors away from the
setting in which research is conducted, as in laboratory studies. However,
there is also much to be gained from laboratory research. Superfluous
contextual factors that may muddle a researchers understanding of a particular
phenomenon can be removed from studies in laboratories.
Psychological experiments, more specifically, introduce
their own costs and benefits. In an experiment, the researcher seeks to
demonstrate the causal effects of one entity (the independent variable) on
another (the dependent variable). When this is done in the laboratory,
contextual factors except for the causal entity need to be minimized or
controlled. Thus, the }all powerful” experimenter attempts to strip away contextual
factors that might add }noise” to the data. Then, typically unbeknownst to the
participants, the experimental environment is manipulated or altered in some
way and then the participants are asked to respond. This manipulation (and
sometimes deception) produces a situation in which a hierarchical relationship
is constructed between the experimenter and the participants. The experimenter
has considerable power over the situation and control over the participants.
This power differential has the potential of leading the participants to feel
exploited or demoralized, which is the antithesis of the outcome that feminist
researchers seek.
Integrating
Research Approaches
Although quantitative methods and laboratory
experimentation have their flaws from a feminist point of view, we contend that
the discipline has nonetheless benefited a great deal from these approaches.
Powerful feminist arguments have been substantiated from their use. For
example, only by quantitative methods is it possible to document the widespread
violence against women on college campuses (e.g., Koss, 1987) and only with
experimental methods can one investigate the direct causal effects of
gender-biased language (Hyde, 1984). To illustrate, one of us (JH) conducted an
experiment to determine whether childrens perceptions of a fictitious
occupation (wudgemaking) were influenced by the pronouns used to describe
people who did this for a living (Hyde, 1984). She found that participants
rated women as only moderately competent at the task when the pronoun was
masculine, but their ratings rose when either a gender-fair or feminine pronoun
was used. The use of the experimental method allowed the researcher to
determine the influence of masculine, feminine, or gender-fair pronouns on
childrens perceptions of this fictitious occupation. It is also noteworthy that
although the children in this study were in an experiment, they most likely
were not led to feel manipulated or controlled.
Issues concerning hierarchy and control can emerge in
almost any research setting. For example, personal and professional
responsibilities may be blurred in an ongoing interview study, where intimate
information is passed from participant to investigator. Thus, even qualitative,
non-laboratory researchers face ethical challenges in navigating the path
between data collection and interpersonal exploitation or invasion of privacy.
Laboratory experimentation, however, has been most highly criticized by
feminists. First, to provide a balanced view, it is notable that participants
are at minimal risk in most laboratory experimental research. Furthermore, the
ethics of research procedures are monitored by a designated board of people who
review all proposed research with human participants, so long as the research
is conducted within a university context (a practice that also is followed with
research that is not experimental). In addition, recognizing the usefulness of
laboratory studies, feminists have made strides in improving the situations
into which research participants enter. Landrine, Klonoff, and Brown-Collins
(1992) have made several suggestions of how feminist psychologists can alter
their methods to coincide more with feminist ideals. For example, they
suggested that researchers provide monetary compensation for participants
service and never coerce participation as part of a course requirement.
Moreover, in an ideal world, research would be designed so that the people
under investigation actually benefited from participation.
We suggest that the methods just discussed have different
strengths and weaknesses and compliment each other well. Used in tandem, these
methods may prove to be extremely useful in terms of leading to our greater
understanding of gender and its intersection with other social and cultural phenomena.
For example, an area of research may at first be investigated qualitatively to
determine what topics are important to participants lives. Then, once these
more specific aspects are identified, they can be investigated via quantitative
methods. Qualitative and quantitative methods might also be used iteratively.
That is, the first study on a question might be qualitative, followed by a
quantitative study, followed by a qualitative study designed to clarify the
findings of the quantitative research, and so on.
Deborah Tolman and Laura Szalacha (in press) provided an
excellent example of multiple-method research on adolescent girls experiences
of sexual desire. Narrative data from 30 girls were collected, and then the
data were summarized both qualitatively and quantitatively. The researchers
were able to summarize the data at multiple levels of specificity. They
reported overall patterns using statistical analysis and then returned to the
original narrative data to capture the richness of individual girls experiences
and to explore the nature of the overall patterns. This type of research should
inspire researchers within psychology of women to liberate their research
questions and paradigms from the constraints of rigid boundaries between
methodologies.
As psychology in general continues to progress away from
a positivist perspective (Hare-Mustin & Marecek, 1988), peoples varied
experiences within a given situation will be recognized as important. In this
way, the psychological meaning of a situation, as constructed by participants,
will become more central and thus press the empirical methods used in
psychology to be more diverse and flexible. A challenge to researchers in
psychology of women is to lead the way in this use of multiple methods. This approach
may be more difficult than it seems at first glance. One area in which
attention can be focussed in order to bring this about concerns the training of
scholars in these diverse methods. Typically, one is trained to be either a
quantitative or qualitative researcher. This boundary needs to be broken and
open dialogue should occur between researchers using both approaches. A second
area in which gains need to be made is in the evaluative criteria for
qualitative methods. There is general consensus in psychology about what
methods are acceptable among quantitative approaches (e.g., appropriateness of
statistical tests, adequacy of comparison groups), but this is less true of
qualitative methods. Guidelines have begun to be defined (e.g., Guba & Lincoln,
1989; Olesen, 1994) and these should continue to be developed so that adopting
qualitative methods from other backgrounds will feel more confident in this
methodology.
Finally, given that we embark on our research with a
clear feminist perspective, it is important for the psychology of women that we
make every attempt to communicate to others our message and our research in a
way that others will }listen” to and respond to affirmatively. It is imperative
for feminist psychologists to conduct careful, well-planned research so that
our results will be respected and taken seriously. Only by conveying our
research in a way that is compelling to psychology as a whole will we be
successful in accomplishing our goals and improving the lives of women.
Applications
of Psychology of Women in the Real World
Public
Policy
One of the most neglected public policy issues in the
United States is parental leave, which refers to the leave from work that a
mother or father takes at the time of the birth (or adoption) of a baby (Hyde,
Essex, Clark, Klein, & Byrd, 1996). We offer this issue as an example of
the ways in which psychological research can have an impact in the policy arena.
In 1985, the United States was the only developed nation to have no national
policy providing parental leave for new parents. In 1993, the federal Family
and Medical Leave Act (FMLA) became law. It provides for a minimum leave of 12
weeks for new mothers or fathers. The leave must be job-guaranteed -- that is,
the employee has the right to return to the same job or a comparable one. The
minimum requirement is for unpaid leave, but employers may be more generous and
provide paid leave. These requirements hold only for persons employed by a
company that has 50 or more employees; small businesses are not required to
comply. This policy is considerably less generous than policies in European
nations.
When Congress was considering whether to pass the FMLA,
it called on psychologists to provide expert testimony. The research of experts
-- most of whom were developmental psychologists and testified that infants
need 4 to 6 months with their mother, father, or other stable caregiver in
order to form a secure attachment, which is crucial for later adjustment -- was
instrumental in passing the bill.
Our research team, examining the expert testimony,
noticed that it focused exclusively on infants and their well-being, and that
the well-being of mothers had been ignored. We launched a project, the
Wisconsin Maternity Leave and Health Project, to provide data on this crucial
issue and, we hoped, to inform future public policy decisions (Hyde et al.,
1996; Hyde, Klein, Essex, & Clark, 1995). We found, for example, that a
short leave (6 weeks or less) acts as a risk factor, when combined with other
risk factors such as a troubled marriage, for elevated levels of depressive
symptoms in women. We also found that 66% of the women took a shorter leave
than they would have preferred (Hyde et al., 1996). The reason? Finances. Most
could not afford to stay away from work any longer, when the leave was unpaid.
The results from this project should help to inform decisions about parental
leave policy in the future. They build a strong case for strengthening the
current legislation to provide for paid leave.
Efficacy
of Therapy
One of the hot questions in psychology today is whether
various forms of psychotherapy "work" -- an issue variously known as
the efficacy of therapy or empirically supported therapies (Chambless &
Hollon, 1998; Compas, Haaga, Keefe, Leitenberg, & Williams, 1998). This
question has been asked of feminist therapy and of therapies used to treat
disorders in which a preponderance of the affected people are women, e.g., eating
disorders (e.g., Compas et al., 1998).
Is feminist therapy effective? The bottom line is that we
do not have enough good research to be able to answer the question. Ideally, an
evaluation study should randomly assign clients to feminist therapy or no
therapy (the latter group receives therapy later, after the research is
completed), and then evaluates the clients at the end of therapy compared with
the no-treatment controls. We know of no study that has used such a design. One
of the few relevant studies compared women in feminist therapy with women in
traditional therapy (Marecek, Kravetz, & Finn, 1979). Unfortunately, the
women had selected the therapy themselves rather than being randomly assigned,
so we cannot be certain whether differences between the groups were a result of
the different therapies or pre-existing differences.
Evaluating the effectiveness of feminist therapy will
surely be an important research topic in the next decade. Two distinct
questions can be asked: (a) Is feminist therapy effective compared with no
therapy? and (b) Is feminist therapy as effective or more effective than
traditional therapies? Beyond that, we can ask more complex questions, such as
whether feminist therapy is particularly effective in treating certain problems.
The first question is important and feasible to answer. The second question
probably will not lend itself easily to research because most feminist
therapists incorporate elements of traditional therapies into their treatment,
as appropriate to the particular problem (Enns, 1993). It would therefore be
difficult to separate out the effect of the particular feminist components.
Forensic
Psychology of Women
A substantial number of feminist psychologists -- some of
them academic researchers, some of them feminist therapists -- work in the area
of forensic psychology. This area may include analysis of psychological harm
arising from trauma (e.g., domestic violence, sexual harassment), evaluation of
parents in child custody cases, or preparing an attorney to cross-examine an
accused rapist (for a review, see Brown, in press). Feminist forensic
psychologists develop their expert opinions based on a combination of basic
research in the psychology of women and accumulated clinical experience of
themselves and others in treating women who have experienced such traumas. In
this role, they strongly influence legal outcomes. Examples include judgments
about whether a mother loses custody of her children following a divorce,
whether a woman can sue an employer for damages on account of psychological
trauma she suffered as a result of sexual harassment on the job, and whether an
accused rapist is convicted.
As one specific example, feminist forensic psychologists
gradually accumulated an argument that was upheld in many legal cases.
Specifically, they argued that a battered woman might well be in constant fear
for her life, even if a knife was not held to her throat at a particular
moment; therefore, if she killed her batterer, it could reasonably be regarded
as self-defense (Brown, in press). Feminist forensic psychology is an exciting
area that doubtless will expand in the future.
Advice
to Aspiring Feminist Researchers
One of the most important goals of feminist psychologists
is the improvement of the lives of girls and women and a commitment to that
goal will surely extend into the 21st century. If we are to improve the lives
of girls and women, so many areas desperately need more research and more
talented researchers. We have only begun to glimpse the complex behavioral and
cultural mechanisms that will help us unravel the puzzles before us in feminist
topics, such as sexual assault, sexual harassment, eating disorders,
homophobia, the double whammy of racism and sexism. Without basic research to
give us an understanding of these issues, we cannot hope to be effective in
treating those affected by these problems, much less in preventing them.
If you want to pursue a graduate degree specializing in
feminist psychology, you will need to be especially careful in accepting the
appropriate graduate program. For example, not every department is supportive
of feminist approaches. Some may actively discourage research related to women
and gender. If possible, select a university that is strong in both psychology
and womens studies. Ideally, you should take graduate courses in womens studies
as well as psychology.
Mentorship is also a significant factor in establishing a
successful research program as a feminist psychologist. Interview faculty
members in the department who express an interest in feminist psychology. Find
out which feminist psychologists have graduate student positions available.
Then find out if those individuals would be willing to work with you
specifically. Chances are that feminist mentors may be especially supportive in
helping you survive the rigors of graduate training.
Many entire textbooks have been written about the
psychology of women and gender (e.g., Hyde, 1996). Our coverage here has been
necessarily brief and has omitted much. As we think forward to the 21st
century, we have a solid foundation of three decades of research and theory on
which to build. It is exciting to be part of this effort. Feminist researchers
especially welcome new researchers into the fold who can continue to work
energetically for causes important to girls and women. We think that it is
exciting to be part of this effort.
References
Asch, A., & Sacks, L. (1983). Lives without, lives
within: The autobiographies of blind women and men. Journal of Visual
Impairment and Blindness, 77, 242-247.
Asrael, W. (1982). An approach to motherhood for disabled
women. Rehabilitation Literature, 43, 214-218.
Ault, A. (1996). Ambiguous identity in an unambiguous
sex/gender structure: The case of bisexual women. Sociological Quarterly, 37,
449-463.
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological
androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155-162.
Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive
account of sex-typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354-364.
Blackwood, E. (1984). Sexuality and gender in certain
Native American tribes: The case of the cross-gender cultures. Signs: Journal
of Women in Culture and Society, 10, 27-42.
Boston Lesbian Psychologies Collective (1987). Lesbian
psychologies: Explorations and challenges. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois
Press.
Brand, P. A., & Kidd, A. H. (1986). Frequency of
physical aggression in heterosexual and female homosexual dyads. Psychological
Reports, 59, 1307-1313.
Brown, L. (in press). Feminist ethical considerations in
forensic practice. In M. Brabeck (Ed.), Feminist ethics in psychology.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining
empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
66, 7-18.
Compas, B. E., Haaga, D. A., Keefe, F. J., Leitenberg,
H., & Williams, D. A. (1998). Sampling of empirically supported
psychological treatments from health psychology: Smoking, chronic pain, cancer,
and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 89-112.
Davis, N. D., Cole, E., & Rothblum, E. D. (Eds.)
(1996). Lesbian therapists and their therapy: From both sides of the couch. New
York: Harrington Park Press.
Deutsch, H. (1944). The psychology of women. New York:
Grune & Stratton.
Enns, C. Z. (1993). Twenty years of feminist counselling
and therapy: From naming biases to implementing multifaceted practice. The
Counselling Psychologist, 21, 3-87.
Fine, M., & Asch, A. (1988). Women with disabilities:
Essays in psychology, culture, and politics. Philadelphia: Temple University
Press.
Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact
of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621-628.
Flaks, D. K., Ficher, I., Masterpasqua, F., & Joseph,
G. (1995). Lesbians choosing motherhood: A comparative study of lesbians and
heterosexual parents and their children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 105-114.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a difference voice: Psychological
theory and womens development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press.
Greene, B. & Herek, G. M. (Eds.) (1994). Lesbian and
gay psychology: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Thousand oaks, CA:
Sage.
Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1989). Fourth
generation evaluation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Hare-Mustin, R. T. & Marecek, J. (1988). The meaning
of difference: Gender theory, post modernism and psychology. American
Psychologist, 43, 455-464.
Heilbrun, C. G. (1973). Toward a recognition of
androgyny. New York: Knopf.
Hyde, J. S. (1984). Childrens understanding of sexist
language. Developmental Psychology, 20, 697-706.
Hyde, J. S. (1996). Half the human experience: The
psychology of women. 5th ed., Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Hyde, J. S., Essex, M. J., Clark, R., Klein, M. H., &
Byrd, J. E. (1996). Parental leave: Policy and research. Journal of Social
Issues, 52(3), 91-109.
Hyde, J. S., Klein, M. H., Essex, M. J., & Clark, R.
(1995). Maternity leave and women's mental health. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 19, 257-285.
Jaggar, A. M. & Rothenberg, P. S. (1993). Feminist
frameworks: Alternative accounts of the relations between women and men. 3rd
ed., New York: McGraw-Hill.
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental
approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization
theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Koss, M. P. (1987). The scope of rape: Incidence and
prevalence in a national sample of higher education students. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 162-170.
Landrine, H., Klonoff, E. Z., & Brown-Collins, A.
(1992). Cultural diversity and methodology in feminist psychology: Critique,
proposal, empirical example. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16, 145-163.
Malamuth, N. M., Sockoskie, R. J., Koss, M. P., &
Tanaka, J. S. (1991). Characteristics of aggressors against women: Testing a
model using a national sample of college students. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 59, 670-681.
Marecek, J., Kravetz, D., & Finn, S. (1979).
Comparison of women who enter feminist therapy and women who enter traditional
therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 734-742.
Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1983). The effects
of sex-typing schemas on young children's memory. Child Development, 54,
563-574.
McKinley, N. M. (1995). Women and objectified body
consciousness: A feminist psychological analysis. (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Wisconsin--Madison, Madison, WI, 1995), Dissertation Abstracts
International, 56, 05B, 9527111.
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The
Objectified Body Consciousness Scale: Development and validation. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 20, 181-215.
Olesen, V. (1994). Feminism and models of qualitative
research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative
research (pp. 158-174). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Patterson, C. (1992). Children of lesbian and gay
parents. Child Development, 63, 1025-1042.
Peplau, L. A., Cochran, S., Rock, K., & Padesky, C.
(1978). Loving women: Attachment and autonomy in lesbian relationships. Journal
of Social Issues, 34, 7-27.
Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1981). Androgyny
versus gender schema: A comment on Bem's gender schema theory. Psychological
Review, 88, 365-368.
Spitzack, C. (1990). Confessing excess: Women and the
politics of body reduction. Albany, NY: state University of New York Press.
Tolman, D. L., & Szalacha, L. A. (in press).
Dimensions of desire: bridging qualitative and quantitative methods in a study
of female adolescent sexuality. Psychology of Women Quarterly
Tong, R. (1989). Feminist thought: A comprehensive
introduction. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Pryor, D. W.
(1994). Dual attraction: Understanding bisexuality. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Weinstock, J. S. & Rothblum, E. D. (Eds.) (1996).
Lesbian friendships: For ourselves and each other. New York: New York
University Press.
Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social
comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231j248.
Post a Comment