How Our Sense of Touch Is a Lot Like the Way We Hear.
One of the New research explains “How Our Sense of Touch
Is a Lot Like the Way We Hear”. When you walk into a darkened room, your first
instinct is to feel around for a light switch. You slide your hand along the
wall, feeling the transition from the doorframe to the painted drywall, and
then up and down until you find the metal or plastic plate of the switch.
During the process you use your sense of touch to develop an image in your mind
of the wall's surface and make a better guess for where the switch is.
Sliman Bensmaia, PhD, assistant professor of organismal
biology and anatomy at the University of Chicago, studies the neural basis of
tactile perception, or how our hands convey this information to the brain. In a
new study published in the Journal of Neuroscience, he and his colleagues found
that the timing and frequency of vibrations produced in the skin when you run
your hands along a surface, like searching a wall for a light switch, play an
important role in how we use our sense of touch to gather information about the
objects and surfaces around us.
The sense of touch has traditionally been thought of in
spatial terms, i.e. receptors in the skin are spread out across a grid of
sorts, and when you touch something this grid of receptors transmits
information about the surface to your brain. In their new study, Bensmaia, two
former undergraduates, and a postdoctoral scholar in his lab -- Matthew Best,
Emily Mackevicius and Hannes Saal -- found that the skin is also highly
sensitive to vibrations, and that these vibrations produce corresponding
oscillations in the afferents, or nerves, that carry information from the
receptors to the brain. The precise timing and frequency of these neural
responses convey specific messages about texture to the brain, much like the
frequency of vibrations on the eardrum conveys information about sound.
Neurons communicate through electrical bits, similar to
the digital ones and zeros used by computers. But, Bensmaia said, "One of
the big questions in neuroscience is whether it's just the number of bits that
matters, or if the specific sequence of bits in time also plays a role. What we
show in this paper is that the sequence of bits in time does matter, and in
fact for some of the skin receptors, the timing matters with millisecond
precision."
Researchers have known for years that these afferents
respond to skin vibrations, but they studied their responses using so-called
sinusoidal waves, which are smooth, repetitive patterns. These perfectly
uniform vibrations can be produced in a lab, but the kinds of vibrations
produced in the skin by touching surfaces in the real world are messy and
erratic.
For this study, Bensmaia and his team used a vibratory
motor that can produce any complex vibration they want. In the first
experiment, they recorded afferent responses to a variety of frequencies in
rhesus macaques, whose tactile nervous system closely resembles humans. In the
second part, a group of human subjects reported how similar or different two
particular frequencies felt when a probe attached to the motor touched their
skin.
When the team analyzed the data recorded from the rhesus
macaques, they found that not only did the nerve oscillate at the frequency of
the vibrations, but they could also predict how the human subjects would
perceive vibrations based on the neuronal responses to the same frequencies in
the macaques.
"In this paper, we showed that the timing of spikes
evoked by naturalistic vibrations matters, not just for artificial stimuli in
the lab," Bensmaia said. "It's actually true for the kinds of stimuli
that you would experience in everyday life."
What this means is that given a certain texture, we know
the frequency of vibrations it will produce in the skin, and subsequently in
the nerve. In other words, if you knew the frequency of silk as your finger
passes over it, you could reproduce the feeling by stimulating the nerves with
that same frequency without ever touching the fabric.
But this study is just part of ongoing research for
Bensmaia's team on how humans incorporate our sense of touch into more
sophisticated concepts like texture, shape, and motion.
Researchers could someday use this model of timing and
frequency of afferent responses to simulate the sensation of texture for an
amputee by "replaying" the vibrations produced in an artificial limb
as it explores a textured surface by electrically stimulating the nerve at the
corresponding frequencies. It could also be used for haptic rendering, or
producing the tactile feel of a virtual object on a touchscreen (think turning
your iPad into a device for reading Braille, or controlling robotic surgery).
"We're trying to build a theory of what makes things
feel the way they feel," Bensmaia said. "This is the beginning of a
story that's really going to change the way people think about the
somatosensory system."
[image source: photostock.com]
Source: sciencedaily
Psychtronics.com gives the only interesting topics of
psychology and you need not to be a professional to understand the articles in
the psychtronics. They are easy to understand to every one and it is mainly for
the college students and Psychiatrists.
Like us in FB to get Updates: www.facebook.com/psychtronics
Follow us in twitter: www.twitter.com/psychtronics
Post a Comment